Illusions in Flight
Visual Illusions, Vestibular Illusions
An illusion is a false percept. An orientational
illusion is a false percept of one's position or motion--either linear
or angular--relative to the plane of the earth's surface. A great number
of orientational illusions occur during flight: some named, others unnamed;
some understood, others not understood. Those that are sufficiently impressive
to cause pilots to report them, whether because of their repeatability
or because of their emotional impact, have been described in the aeromedical
literature and will be discussed here. The illusions in flight are categorized
into those resulting from visual misperceptions and those involving vestibular
errors.
Visual
Illusions
Shape Constancy, Size Constancy Aerial Perspective, Absent Focal Cues, Absent Ambient Cues, Autokinesis Vection Illusions, False Horizons and Surface Planes, Other False Ambient Cues
We
shall organize the visual illusions in flight according to whether they
involve primarily the focal mode of visual processing or primarily the
ambient mode. Although this categorization is somewhat arbitrary and
may seem too coarse in some cases, it serves to emphasize the dichotomous
nature of visual orientation information processing. We begin with illusions
involving primarily focal vision. (Many of the visual illusions related
in this section were first described by Pitts.23)
Shape Constancy
To appreciate how false shape constancy
cueing can create orientational illusions in flight, consider the example
provided by a runway that is constructed on other than level terrain.
Figure 14a shows the pilot's view of the runway during an approach to
landing and demonstrates the linear perspective and foreshortening of
the runway that the pilot associates with a 3° approach slope. If the
runway slopes upward 1° (a rise of only 35 m in a 2-km runway), the
foreshortening of the runway for a pilot on a 3° approach slope is substantially
less (the height of the retinal image of the runway is greater) than
it would be if the runway were level. This can give the pilot the illusion
of being too high on the approach. The pilot's natural response to such
an illusion is to reshape the image of the runway by seeking a shallower
approach slope (Fig. 14b). This response, of course, could be hazardous.
The opposite situation results when the runway slopes downward. To perceive
the accustomed runway shape under this condition, the pilot flies a
steeper approach slope than usual (Fig. 14c).
Figure 14. Effect
of runway slope on the pilot's image of runway during final approach (left)
and potential effect on the approach slope angle flown (right). a.
Flat runway -- normal approach. b. An up-sloping runway creates the illusion
of being high on approach -- pilot flies the approach too low. c. A down-sloping
runway has the opposite effect.
Size
Constancy
Size constancy is very important in judging
distance, and false cues are frequently responsible for aircraft mishaps
due to illusions of focal visual origin. The runway width illusions
are particularly instructive in this context. Figure 15a shows the accustomed
runway width and a normal approach. A runway that is narrower than that
to which a pilot is accustomed can create a hazardous illusion on the
approach to landing. Size constancy causes the pilot to perceive the
narrow runway to be farther away (i.e., the aircraft is higher) than
is actually the case; hence, the pilot may flare too late and touch
fly a steeper approach than if the approach terrain were level (Fig.
16a). If the approach terrain slopes up to the runway, on the other
hand, the pilot tends to fly a less steep approach (Fig. 16b). Although
the estimation of height above the approach terrain depends on both
focal and ambient vision, the contribution of focal vision is particularly
clear: consider the pilot who looks at a building below and, seeing
it to be closer than such buildings usually are, seeks a higher approach
slope. By the same token, focal vision and size constancy are responsible
for poor height and distance judgments pilots sometimes make when flying
over terrain having an unfamiliar composition (Fig. 17). A reported
example of this is the tendency to misjudge the approach height when
landing in the Aleutians, where the evergreen trees are much smaller
than those to which most pilots are accustomed. Such height-estimation
difficulties are by no means restricted to the approach and landing
phases of flight. One fatal mishap occurred during air combat training
over the Southwest desert when the pilot of a high-performance fighter
aircraft presumably misjudged the aircraft's height over the desert
floor because of the small, sparse vegetation and was unable to arrest
a deliberate descent to a ground-hugging altitude.
Figure 15.Effect
of runway width on the pilot's image of runway (left) and the potential
effect on approach flown (right). a. Accustomed width--normal approach.
b. A narrow runway makes the pilot feel the aircraft is higher than it
actually is, which results in the approach being too low and the flare
being too late. c. A wide runway gives the illusion of being closer than
it actually is--the pilot tends to approach too high and flares too soon.
Figure 16. Potential
effect of the slope of the terrain under the approach on the approach
slope flown. a. The terrain slopes down to the runway; the pilot thinks
the approach is too shallow and steepens it. b. Upsloping terrain makes
the pilot think the aircraft is too high, which is corrected by making
the approach too shallow.
Aerial
Perspective
Aerial perspective also can play a role
in deceiving the pilot, and the approach-to-landing regime again provides
examples. In daytime, fog or haze can make a runway appear farther away
as a result of the loss of visual discrimination. At night, runway and
approach lights in fog or rain appear less bright than they do in clear
weather and can create the illusion that they are farther away. It has
even been reported that a pilot can have an illusion of being banked
to the right, for example, if the runway lights are brighter on the
right side of the runway than they are on the left. Another hazardous
illusion of this type can occur during approach to landing in a shallow
fog or haze, especially during a night approach. The vertical visibility
under such conditions is much better than the horizontal visibility,
so that descent into the fog causes the more distant approach or runway
lights to diminish in intensity at the same time that the peripheral
visual cues are suddenly occluded by the fog. The result is an illusion
that the aircraft has pitched up, with the concomitant danger of a nose-down
corrective action by the pilot.
Absent Focal Cues
A well-known pair of approach-to-landing
situations that create illusions because of the absence of adequate
focal visual orientation cues are the smooth-water (or glassy-water)
and snow-covered approaches. In a seaplane, one's perception of height
is degraded substantially when the water below is still. For that reason,
a seaplane pilot routinely just sets up a safe descent rate and waits
for the seaplane to touch down, rather than attempting to flare to a
landing when the water is smooth. A blanket of fresh snow on the ground
and runway also deprives the pilot of visual cues with which to estimate
height above ground, thus making the approach extremely difficult. Again,
approaches are not the only regime in which smooth water and fresh snow
cause problems. A number of aircraft have crashed as a result of pilots
maneuvering over smooth water or snow-covered ground and misjudging
their height above the surface.
Absent Ambient Cues
Two runway approach conditions that create
considerable difficulty for the pilot, by requiring focal vision alone
to accomplish what is normally accomplished with both focal and ambient
vision, are the black-hole and whiteout approaches. A black-hole approach
is one that is made on a dark night over water or unlighted terrain
to a runway beyond which the horizon is indiscernible, the worst case
being when only the runway lights are visible (Fig. 18). Without peripheral
visual cues to help orient the aircraft relative to the earth, the pilot
tends to feel that the aircraft is stable and situated appropriately
but that the runway itself moves about or remains malpositioned (is
down-sloping, for example). Such illusions make the black-hole approach
difficult and dangerous, and often result in a landing far short of
the runway. A particularly hazardous type of black-hole approach is
one made under conditions wherein the earth is totally dark except for
the runway and the lights of a city situated on rising terrain beyond
the runway. Under these conditions, the pilot may try to maintain a
constant vertical visual angle for the distant city lights, thus causing
the aircraft to arc far below the intended approach slope as it gets
closer to the runway (Fig. 19).24 An alternative explanation is that the pilot falsely perceives through
ambient vision that the rising terrain is flat, which leads to a lower-than-normal
approach.
An approach made under whiteout conditions
can be as difficult as a black-hole approach, and for essentially the
same reason--lack of sufficient ambient visual orientation cues. There
are actually two types of whiteout, the atmospheric whiteout and the
blowing-snow whiteout. In the atmospheric whiteout, a snow-covered ground
merges with a white overcast, creating a condition in which ground textural
cues are absent and the horizon is indistinguishable. Although visibility
may be unrestricted in the atmospheric whiteout, there is essentially
nothing to see except the runway or runway markers; an approach made
in this condition must therefore be accomplished with a close eye on
the altitude and attitude instruments to prevent spatial disorientation
and inadvertent ground contact. In the blowing-snow whiteout, visibility
is restricted drastically by snowflakes, and often those snowflakes
have been driven into the air by the propeller or rotor wash of the
affected aircraft. Helicopter landings on snow-covered ground are particularly
likely to create blowing-snow whiteouts. Typically, the helicopter pilot
tries to maintain visual contact with the ground during the sudden rotor-induced
whiteout, gets into an unrecognized drift to one side, and shortly thereafter
contacts the ground with sufficient lateral motion to cause the craft
to roll over. Pilots flying where whiteouts can occur must be made aware
of the hazards of whiteout approaches, as the disorientation induced
usually occurs unexpectedly under visual rather than instrument meteorological
conditions.
Another condition in which a pilot is apt
to make a serious misjudgment is in closing on another aircraft at high
speed. When the pilot has numerous peripheral visual cues by which to
establish both the aircraft's position and velocity relative to the
earth and the target's position and velocity relative to the earth,
the difficulty of tracking and closing is not much different from what
it would be on the ground giving chase to a moving quarry. When relative
position and closure rate cues must come from foveal vision alone, however--as
is generally the case at altitude, at night, or under other conditions
of reduced visibility--the tracking and closing problem is much more
difficult.
Figure 18. Effect
of loss of ambient orientation cues on the perception of runway orientation
during a black-hole approach. a. When ambient orientation cues are
absent, the pilot feels horizontal and (in this example) perceives the
runway to be tilted left and upsloping. b. With the horizon visible, the
pilot can orient correctly with peripheral vision and the runway appears
horizontal in central vision.
An overshoot,
or worse, a midair collision, can easily result from the perceptual
difficulties inherent in such circumstances, especially when the pilot
lacks experience in an environment devoid of peripheral visual cues.
Figure 19. A common and particularly
dangerous type of black-hole approach, in which the pilot falsely perceives
the distant city to be on flat terrain and arcs
below the desired approach slope
A related phenomenon
that pilots need especially to be aware of is the dip illusion. It occurs
during formation flying at night, when one aircraft is in trail behind
another. To avoid wake turbulence and maintain sight of the lead aircraft,
the pilot in trail flies at a small but constant angle below the lead
aircraft by placing the image of the lead aircraft in a particular position
on the windscreen and keeping it there. Now suppose the pilot is told
to "take spacing" (separate) to 5 nautical miles (10 km). For every 1°
below lead the trailing pilot flies, he or she is lower than lead by 1.7%
(sin 1°) of the distance behind lead. Thus, if the trail pilot is 2° below
lead and keeps the image of the lead aircraft at the same spot on the
windscreen all the way back to 5 miles, the trail aircraft will descend
to about 1100 ft (350 m) below the lead aircraft. To make matters worse,
when the aircraft in trail slows down to establish separation, its pitch
attitude increases by several degrees, and if the pilot does not compensate
for this additional angle and tries to maintain the lead aircraft image
in the same relative position, the altitude difference between the two
aircraft can be doubled or even tripled. In the absence of ambient visual
orientation cues, the pilot cannot detect the large loss of altitude without
monitoring the flight instruments, and may inadvertently "dip" far below
the intended flight path. Clearly this situation would be extremely hazardous
if it were to occur at low altitude or during maneuvers in which altitude
separation from other aircraft is critical.
One puzzling illusion
that occurs when ambient visual orientation cues are minimal is visual
autokinesis (Fig. 20). A small, dim light seen against a dark background
is an ideal stimulus for producing autokinesis. After 6 to 12 seconds
of visually fixating the light, one can observe it to move at up to 20°
/sec in a particular direction or in several directions in succession,
but there is little apparent displacement of the object fixated. In general,
the larger and brighter the object, the less the autokinetic effect. The
physiologic mechanism of visual autokinesis is not entirely understood.
One suggested explanation for the autokinetic phenomenon is that the eyes
tend to drift involuntarily, perhaps because of inadequate or inappropriate
vestibular stabilization and that checking the drift requires efferent
oculomotor activity having sensory correlates that create the illusion.
Whatever the mechanism,
the effect of visual autokinesis on pilots is of some importance. Anecdotes
abound of pilots who fixate a star or a stationary ground light at night,
and seeing it move because of autokinesis, mistake it for another aircraft
and try to intercept or join up with it. Another untoward effect of the
illusion occurs when a pilot flying at night perceives a relatively stable
tracked aircraft to be moving erratically when in fact it is not; the
unnecessary and undesirable control inputs the pilot makes to compensate
for the illusory movement of the target aircraft represent increased work
and wasted motion at best and an operational hazard at worst.
To help avoid or reduce
the autokinetic illusion, the pilot should try to maintain a well-structured
visual environment in which spatial orientation is unambiguous. Because
this is rarely possible in night flying, it has been suggested that (1)
the pilot's gaze should be shifted frequently to avoid prolonged fixation
of a target light; (2) the target should be viewed beside, through, or
in some other reference to a relatively stationary structure such as a
canopy bow; (3) the pilot should make eye, head, and body movements to
try to destroy the illusion; and (4) as always, the pilot should monitor
the flight instruments to help prevent or resolve any perceptual conflict.
Equipping aircraft with more than one light or with luminescent strips
to enhance recognition at night probably has helped reduce problems with
autokinesis.
Figure
20. Visual autokinesis. A small, solitary light or small group of lights
seen in the dark can appear to move, when in fact they are stationary.
So far, this section
has dealt with visual illusions created by excessive orientation-processing
demands being placed on focal vision when adequate orientation cues are
not available through ambient vision or when strong but false orientation
cues are received through focal vision. Ambient vision can itself be responsible
for creating orientational illusions, however, whenever orientation cues
received in the visual periphery are misleading or misinterpreted. Probably
the most compelling of such illusions are the vection illusions. Vection
is the visually induced perception of self-motion in the spatial environment
and can be a sensation of linear self-motion (linear vection) or angular
self-motion (angular vection).
Nearly everyone who
drives an automobile has experiencedone very common linear vection illusion:
when we are stopped at a stoplight and a large, presumably stationary
vehicle in the adjacent lane creeps forward, a compelling illusion that
our own car is creeping backward can result (prompting a swift but surprisingly
ineffectual stomp on the brakes). Similarly, when one is sitting in a
stationary train and the train on the adjacent track begins to move, a
strong sensation that one's own train is moving in the opposite direction
can be experienced (Fig. 21a). Linear vection is one of the things that
make close formation flying so difficult, because the pilot can never
be sure whether his or her own aircraft or that of the lead or wingman
is responsible for the perceived relative motion.
Angular vection occurs when peripheral
visual cues convey the information that one is rotating; the perceived
rotation can be in pitch, roll, yaw, or other plane. Although angular
vection illusions are not common in life, they can be generated readily
in a laboratory by enclosing a subject in a rotating striped drum. Usually
within 10 seconds after the motion begins, the subject perceives that
he or she rather than the drum is rotating. A pilot can experience angular
vection if the anticollision light on the aircraft is left on during
flight through clouds or the revolving reflection provides a strong
ambient visual stimulus, rotation in the yaw plane.
Another example of vection illusions is
the so-called "Star Wars" named after the popular motion picture famous
for its vection-inducing effects. This phenomenon involves linearly
and angularly moving of ground lights off the curved inside surface
of a fighter aircraft which create in the pilot disconcerting sensations
of motion that conflict with the actual motion of the aircraft.
Fortunately, vection illusions are not
all bad. The most advanced simulators depend on linear and angular vection
to create the illusion of angular motion
(Fig. 21b). When the visual flight environment is dynamically portrayed
by flight simulators with a wide field-of-view and infinity-optics,
the illusion of actual flight
is so compelling that additional mechanical motion is often not even needed (although mechanically generated motion-onset
cues do seem to improve the fidelity of the simulation).
False
Horizons and Surface Planes
Sometimes the horizon perceived through
ambient vision is not horizontal. Quite naturally, this misperception
of the horizontal creates illusions in flight. A sloping cloud deck, for example, is very difficult
to perceive as anything
but horizontal if it extends for any great distance into the peripheral
visual field (Fig. 22). Uniformly sloping terrain, particularly featureless terrain, can also create an illusion
of horizontality with disastrous results,
for the pilot thus deceived. Many aircraft have crashed as a result
of pilot's entering a canyon with an apparently level floor, only to
find that floor actually rose faster than the airplane could climb.
At night, the lights of a city built on sloping terrain can create the
false impression that the extended plane of the city lights
is the horizontal plane of the earth's surface as already noted (Fig.
19). A distant rain shower can obscure the real horizon and create the
impression of a horizon at the proximal edge (base) of the rainfall. If
the shower is seen just beyond the runway during an approach to landing,
the pilot can misjudge the pitch attitude of the aircraft and make inappropriate
pitch corrections on the approach.
Figure
21. Vection illusions. a. Linear vection. In this example, the adjacent
vehicle seen moving aft in the subject's peripheral vision causes the
sensation of moving forward. b. Angular vection. Objects seen revolving
around the subject in the flight simulator result in a perception of self-rotation
in the opposite direction--in this case, a rolling motion to the right.
Pilots are especially susceptible to misperception
of the horizontal while flying at night (Figs. 23a and 23b). Isolated
ground lights can appear to the pilot as stars, creating the perception
of a nose-high or one-wing-low attitude. Flying under such a false impression
can, of course, be fatal. Frequently, no stars are visible because of
overcast conditions. Unlighted areas of terrain can then blend with
the dark overcast to create the illusion that the unlighted terrain
is part of the sky. One extremely hazardous situation is that in which
a takeoff is made over an ocean or other large body of water that cannot
be distinguished visually from the night sky. Many pilots in this situation
have perceived the shoreline receding beneath them to be the horizon,
and some have responded to this false percept with disastrous nose-down
control input.
Figure
22. A sloping cloud deck, which the pilot misperceives as a horizontal
surface.
Pilots flying at
high altitudes can sometimes experience difficulties in control of aircraft
attitude, because at high altitudes the horizon is lower with respect
to the plane of level flight than it is at the lower altitudes most pilots
are accustomed to flying. As a reasonable approximation, angle of depression
of the horizon in degrees equals the square root of altitude in kilometers.
A pilot flying at an altitude of 49,000 ft (15 km) sees the horizon almost
4° below the extension of the horizontal plane of aircraft. By visually
orienting to the view from the left cockpit window, a pilot might be inclined
to fly with the left wing 4° down to level it with horizon. If the pilot
does this and then looks out the right window, the wing would be seen
8° above the horizon, with half of that elevation due to the erroneous
control input. The pilot also might experience problems with pitch control
because the depressed horizon can cause a false perception of 4° nose-high
pitch attitude.
Figure 23. Misperception of the horizontal
at night. a. Ground lights appearing to be stars cause the
earth and sky to blend and a false horizon to be perceived. b. Blending
of overcast sky with unlighted terrain or water causes the horizon to
appear lower than is actually the case.
Other False Ambient
Cues
One very important aspect of ambient visual
orientational cuing is in the stabilizing effect of the surrounding
instrument panel, glare shield, canopy bow or windshield frame, and especially
the reflections of panel and other cockpit structures off the windshield
or canopy at night. The stable visual provided by these objects tends
to cause the aircraft motion to appear not above the threshold for vestibular
motion though it may be well above the usual threshold for vestibular
motion. While flying at night or in instrument weather, a pilot may thus
have a sense of security because no motion is felt, due to the apparently
ambient visual environment. Of course, this falsely stabilizing effect
occurs when the visual environment contains the usually valid ambient
references (natural horizon, earth's surface, etc.).
Another result of false ambient visual
orientational cuing is the sun illusion. On the ground, we are accustomed
to seeing brighter surroundings above and darker below, regardless
of the position of the sun. The direction of this gradient in light intensity
thus helps us orient with respect to the surface of the earth. In clouds,
however, such a gradient generally does not exist, and when it does, the
lighter direction is generally toward the sun and the darker direction
is away from it. But the sun is not always directly overhead; as a consequence,
a pilot flying in a thin cloud layer may perceive falsely that the sun
is directly overhead. This prompts the pilot to bank in the direction
of the sun, hence the name of this illusion.
Finally, the disorienting effects of the
northern lights and of aerial flares should be mentioned. Aerial
refueling at night in high northern latitudes often is made quite difficult
by the northern lights, which provide false cues of verticality to the
pilot's peripheral vision. Similarly, when aerial flares are dropped,
they may drift with the wind, creating false cues of verticality. Their
motion also may create vection illusions. Another phenomenon associated
with use of aerial flares at night is the "moth" effect. The size of the
area on the ground illuminated by a dropped flare slowly decreases as
the flare descends. Because of the size constancy mechanism of visual
orientation discussed earlier, a pilot circling the illuminated area may
tend to fly in a descending spiral with gradually decreasing radius. Another
important factor is that the aerial flares can be so bright as to reduce
the apparent intensity of the aircraft instrument displays and thereby
minimize their orientational cuing strength.
Somatogyral Illusion, Oculogyral
Illusion, Coriolis
Illusion, Somatogravic
Illusion, Inversion
Illusion, G-Excess
Effect, Oculogravic
Illusions, The Leans
The vestibulocerebellar axis processes
orientation information from the vestibular, visual, and other sensory
systems. In the absence of adequate ambient visual orientation cues,
the inadequacies of the vestibular and other orienting senses can result
in orientational illusions. It is convenient and conventional to discuss
the vestibular illusions in relation to the two functional components
of the labyrinth that generate them--the semicircular ducts and the
otolith organs.
Somatogyral Illusion
A somatogyral illusion is a false
sensation of rotation (or absence of rotation) that results from misperceiving
the magnitude or direction of an actual rotation. In essence, somatogyral
illusions result from the inability of the semicircular ducts to register
accurately a prolonged rotation, i.e., sustained angular velocity. When
a person is subjected to an angular acceleration about the yaw axis, for
example, the angular motion is at first perceived accurately because the
dynamics of the cupula-endolymph system cause it to respond as an integrating
angular accelerometer (i.e., as a rotation-rate sensor) at stimulus frequencies
in the physiologic range (Fig. 24). If the acceleration is followed immediately
by a deceleration, as usually happens in the terrestrial environment,
the total sensation of turning one way and then stopping the turn is quite
accurate (Fig. 25). If, however, the angular acceleration is not followed
by a deceleration and a constant angular velocity results instead, the
sensation of rotation becomes less and less and eventually disappears
as the cupula gradually returns to its resting position in the absence
of an angular acceleratory stimulus (Fig. 26). If we are subsequently
subjected to an angular deceleration after a period of prolonged constant
angular velocity, say after 10 seconds or so of constant-rate turning,
our cupula-endolymph systems signal a turn in the direction opposite that
of the prolonged constant angular velocity, even though we are really
only turning less rapidly in the same direction. This is because the angular
momentum of the rotating endolymph causes it to press against the cupula,
forcing the cupula to deviate in the direction of endolymph flow, which
is the same direction the cupula would deviate if we were to accelerate
in the direction opposite the initial acceleration. Even after rotation
actually ceases, the sensation of rotation in the direction opposite that
of the sustained angular velocity persists for several seconds--half a
minute or longer with a large decelerating rotational impulse. Another,
more mechanistic, definition of the somatogyral illusion is "any discrepancy
between actual and perceived rate of self-rotation that results from an
abnormal angular acceleratory stimulus pattern." The term "abnormal" in
this case implies the application of low-frequency stimuli outside the
useful portion of the transfer characteristics of the semicircular duct
system.
In flight under conditions of reduced
visibility, somatogyral illusions can be deadly. The graveyard spin is
the classic example of how somatogyral illusions can disorient a pilot
with fatal results. This situation begins with the pilot intentionally
or unintentionally entering a spin, let's say to the left (Fig. 27). At
first, the pilot perceives the spin correctly because the angular acceleration
associated with entering the spin deviates the cupulae the appropriate
amount in the appropriate direction. The longer the spin persists, however,
the more the sensation of spinning to the left diminishes as the cupulae
return to their resting positions. On trying to stop the spin to the left
by applying the right rudder, the angular deceleration causes the pilot
to perceive a spin to the right, even though the only real result of this
action was termination of the spin to the left. A pilot who is ignorant
of the possibility of such an illusion is then likely to make counterproductive
left-rudder inputs to negate the unwanted erroneous sensation of spinning
to the right. These inputs keep the airplane spinning to the left, which
gives the pilot the desired sensation of not spinning but does not bring
the airplane under control. To extricate himself from this very hazardous
situation, the pilot must read the aircraft flight instruments and apply
control inputs to make the instruments give the desired readings (push
right rudder to center the turn needle, in this example). Unfortunately,
this may not be so easy to do. The angular accelerations created by both
the multiple-turn spin and the pilot's spin-recovery attempts can elicit
strong but inappropriate vestibulo-ocular reflexes, including nystagmus.
In the usual terrestrial environment, these reflexes help stabilize the
retinal image of the visual surround; in this situation, however, they
only destabilize the retinal image because the visual surround (cockpit)
is already fixed with respect to the pilot. Reading the flight instruments
thus becomes difficult or impossible, and the pilot is left with only
false sensations of rotation to rely on for spatial orientation and aircraft
control.26
Figure 24. Transfer characteristics
of the semicircular duct system as a function of sinusoidal stimulus frequency. Gain is the ratio of the magnitude of the peak perceived angular velocity
to the peak delivered angular velocity; phase angle is a measure of the
amount of advance or delay between the peak perceived and peak delivered
angular velocities. Note that in the physiologic frequency range (roughly
0.05 to 1 Hz), perception is accurate; that is, gain is close to unity
(0 dB) and phase angle is minimal. At lower stimulus frequencies, however,
the gain drops off rapidly, and the phase shift approaches 90°, which
means that angular velocity becomes difficult to detect and that angular
acceleration is perceived as velocity. (Adapted from Peters.25)
Figure 25. Effect of the stimulus
pattern on the perception of angular velocity. On the left, the high-frequency
character of the applied angular acceleration results in a cupular deviation
that is nearly identical to, and a perceived angular velocity that is
nearly identical to, the angular velocity developed. On the right, the
peak angular velocity developed is the same as that on the left, but the
low-frequency character of the applied acceleration results in cupular
deviation and perceived angular velocity that appear more like the applied
acceleration than the resulting velocity. This causes one to perceive:
(a) less than the full amount of the angular velocity, (b) absence of
rotation while turning persists, (c) a turn in the opposite direction
from that of the actual turn, and (d) that turning persists after it has
actually stopped. These false percepts are somatogyral illusions.
Although the lore of early aviation
provided the graveyard spin as an illustration of the hazardous nature
of somatogyral illusions, a much more common example occurring all too
often in modern aviation is the graveyard spiral (Fig. 28). In this situation,
the pilot has intentionally or unintentionally entered into a prolonged
turn with a moderate amount of bank. After a number of seconds in the
turn, the pilot loses the sensation of turning because the cupula-endolymph
system cannot respond to constant angular velocity. The percept of being
in a bank as a result of the initial roll into the banked attitude also
decays with time because the net gravitoinertial force vector points toward
the floor of the aircraft during coordinated flight (whether the aircraft
is in a banked turn or flying straight and level), and the otolith organs
and other graviceptors normally signal that down is in the direction of
the net sustained gravitoinertial force. As a result, when trying to stop
the turn by rolling back to a wings-level attitude, the pilot feels not
only a turning in the direction opposite to that of the original turn,
but also a bank in the direction opposite to that of the original bank.
Unwilling to accept this sensation of making the wrong control input,
the hapless pilot rolls back into the original banked turn. Now the pilot's
sensation is compatible with a desired mode of flight, but the flight
instruments indicate a loss of altitude (because the banked turn is wasting
lift) and a continuing turn. So the pilot pulls back on the stick and
perhaps adds power to arrest the unwanted descent and regain the lost
altitude. This action would be successful if the aircraft were flying
wings-level, but with the aircraft in a steeply banked attitude it tightens
the turn, serving only to make matters worse. Unless the pilot eventually
recognizes what is occurring and rolls out of the unperceived banked turn,
the aircraft will continue to descend in an ever-tightening spiral toward
the ground, hence the name graveyard spiral.
Figure 26. Representation of the mechanical
events occurring in a semicircular duct and resulting action potentials
in the associated ampullary nerve during somatogyral illusions. The
angular acceleration pattern applied is that shown in the right side of
Figure 25.
Figure 27. The Graveyard Spin. After
several turns of a spin the pilot begins to lose the sensation of spinning.
When trying to stop the spin, the resulting somatogyral illusion of
spinning in the opposite direction makes the pilot reenter the original
spin. (The solid line indicates actual motion; the dotted line indicates
perceived motion. illusion of spinning in the opposite
direction makes the pilot reenter the original spin. (The solid line
indicates actual motion; the dotted line indicates perceived motion.
Figure
28. The Graveyard Spiral. The pilot in a banked turn loses
the sensation of being banked and turning. Upon trying to establish
a wings-level attitude and stop the turn, the pilot perceives a bank
and a turning in the opposite direction from the original banked turn.
Unable to tolerate the sensation of making an inappropriate control
input, the pilot banks back into the original turn.
Oculogyral Illusion
Whereas a somatogyral illusion is
a false sensation, or lack of sensation, of self-rotation in a subject
undergoing unusual angular motion, an oculogyral illusion is a false sensation
of motion of an object viewed by such a subject.27 For example, if a vehicle with a subject inside is rotating about a vertical
axis at a constant velocity and suddenly stops rotating, the subject experiences
not only a somatogyral illusion of rotation in the opposite direction,
but also an oculogyral illusion of an object in front moving in the opposite
direction. Thus, a somewhat oversimplified definition of the oculogyral
illusion is that it is the visual correlate of the somatogyral illusion;
however, its low threshold and lack of total correspondence with presumed
cupular deviation suggest a more complex mechanism. The attempt to maintain
visual fixation during a vestibulo-ocular reflex elicited by angular acceleration
is probably at least partially responsible for the oculogyral illusion.
In an aircraft during flight at night or in weather, an oculogyral illusion
generally confirms a somatogyral illusion: the pilot who falsely perceives
a turning in a particular direction also observes the instrument panel
to move in the same direction.
Coriolis
Illusion
The vestibular Coriolis effect, also
called the Coriolis cross-coupling effect, vestibular cross-coupling
effect, or simply the Coriolis illusion, is another false percept that
can result from unusual stimulation of the semicircular duct system.
To illustrate this phenomenon, let us consider a subject who has been
rotating in the plane of the horizontal semicircular ducts {roughly
the yaw plane) long enough for the endolymph in those ducts to attain
the same angular velocity as the head: the cupulae in the ampullae of
the horizontal ducts have returned to their resting positions, and the
sensation of rotation has ceased {Fig. 29a). If the subject then nods
forward in the pitch plane, let's say a full 90° for the sake of simplicity,
the horizontal semicircular ducts are removed from the plane of rotation
and the two sets of vertical semicircular ducts are inserted into the
plane of rotation {Fig. 29b). Although the angular momentum of the subject's
rotating head is forcibly transferred at once out of the old plane of
rotation relative to the head, the angular momentum of the endolymph
in the horizontal duct is dissipated more gradually. The torque resulting
from the continuing rotation of the endolymph causes the cupulae in
the horizontal ducts to be deviated, and a sensation of angular motion
occurs in the new plane of the horizontal ducts--now the roll plane
relative to the subject's body. Simultaneously, the endolymph in the
two sets of vertical semicircular ducts must acquire angular momentum
because these ducts have been brought into the plane of constant rotation.
The torque required to impart this change in momentum causes deflection
of the cupulae in the ampullae of these ducts, and a sensation of angular
motion in this plane--the yaw plane relative to the subject's body--results.
The combined effect of the cupular deflection in all three sets of semicircular
ducts is that of a suddenly imposed angular velocity in a plane in which
no actual angular acceleration relative to the subject has occurred.
In the example given, if the original constant-velocity yaw is to the
right and the subject's head pitches forward, the resulting Coriolis
illusion experienced is that of suddenly rolling and yawing to the right.
Figure
29. Mechanism of the Coriolis illusion. A subject
rotating in the yaw plane long enough for the endolymph to stabilize
in the horizontal semicircular duct (a) pitches the head forward (b).
Angular momentum of the endolymph causes the cupula to deviate, and
the subject perceives rotation in the new (i.e., roll) plane of the
horizontal semicircular duct, even though no actual rotation occurred
in that plane.
A particular perceptual
phenomenon experienced occasionally by pilots of relatively high-performance
aircraft during instrument flight has been attributed to the Coriolis
illusion because it occurs in conjunction with large movements of the
head under conditions of prolonged constant angular velocity. It consists
of a sensation of rolling and/or pitching that appears suddenly after
the pilot's attention has been diverted from the instruments in front
and his or her head is moved to view some switches or displays elsewhere
in the cockpit. This illusion is especially deadly because it is most
likely to occur during an instrument approach, a phase of flight in which
altitude is being lost rapidly and cockpit chores (e.g., radio frequency
changes) repeatedly require the pilot to break up his instrument cross-check.
The sustained angular velocities associated with instrument flying are
insufficient to create Coriolis illusions of any great magnitude, however;28 and another mechanism (the G-excess effect) has been proposed to explain
the illusory rotations experienced with head movements in flight.29 Even if not responsible for spatial disorientation in flight, the Coriolis
illusion is useful as a tool to demonstrate the fallibility of our nonvisual
orientation senses. Nearly every military pilot living today has experienced
the Coriolis illusion in the Barany chair or some other rotating device
as part of physiological training, and for most of these pilots it was
then they first realized that their own orientation senses really cannot
be trusted--the most important lesson of all for instrument flying.
Somatogravic
Illusion
The otolith organs are responsible
for a set of illusions known as somatogravic illusions. The mechanism
of illusions of this type involves the displacement of otolithic membranes
on their maculae by inertial forces so as to signal a false orientation
when the resultant gravitoinertial force is perceived as gravitational
(and therefore vertical). Thus, a somatogravic illusion can be defined
as a false sensation of body tilt that results from perceiving as vertical
the direction of a nonvertical gravitoinertial force. The illusion of
pitching up after taking off into conditions of reduced visibility is
perhaps the best illustration of this mechanism. Consider the pilot of
a high-performance aircraft waiting at the end of the runway to take off.
Here, the only force acting on the otolithic membranes is the force of
gravity, and the positions of those membranes on their maculae signal
accurately that down is toward the floor of the aircraft. Suppose the
aircraft now accelerates on the runway, rotates, takes off, cleans up
gear and flaps, and maintains a forward acceleration of 1 g until reaching
the desired climb speed. The 1 G of inertial force resulting from the
acceleration displaces the otolithic membranes toward the back of the
pilot's head. In fact, the new positions of the otolithic membranes are
nearly the same as they would be if the aircraft and pilot had pitched
up 45°, because the new direction of the resultant gravitoinertial force
vector, if one neglects the angle of attack and climb angle, is 45° aft
relative to the gravitational vertical (Fig. 30). Naturally, the pilot's
percept of pitch attitude based on the information from the otolith organs
is one of having pitched up 45°; and the information from nonvestibular
proprioceptive and cutaneous mechanoreceptive senses supports this false
percept, because the sense organs subserving those modalities also respond
to the direction and intensity of the resultant gravitoinertial force.
Given the very strong sensation of a nose-high pitch attitude, one that
is not challenged effectively by the focal visual orientation cues provided
by the attitude indicator, the pilot is tempted to push the nose of the
aircraft down to cancel the unwanted sensation of flying nose-high. Pilots
succumbing to this temptation characteristically crash in a nose-low attitude
a few miles beyond the end of the runway. Sometimes, however, they are
seen to descend out of the overcast nose-low and try belatedly to pull
up, as though they suddenly regained the correct orientation upon seeing
the ground again. Pilots of carrier-launched aircraft need to be especially
wary of the somatogravic illusion. These pilots experience pulse accelerations
lasting 2 to 4 seconds and generating peak inertial forces of +3 to +5
Gx. Although the major acceleration is over quickly, the resulting illusion
of nose-high pitch can persist for half a minute or more afterward, resulting
in a particularly hazardous situation for the pilot who is unaware of
this phenomenon. 30
Figure 30. A somatogravic illusion
occurring on takeoff. The inertial force resulting from the
forward acceleration combines with the force of gravity to create a
resultant gravitoinertial force directed down and aft. The pilot, perceiving
down to be in the direction of the resultant gravitoinertial force,
feels the aircraft is in an excessively nose-high attitude.
Do not be misled by the above example
into believing that only pilots of high-performance aircraft suffer the
somatogravic illusion of pitching up after takeoff. More than a dozen
air transport aircraft are believed to have crashed as a result of the
somatogravic illusion occurring on takeoff.31 A relatively slow aircraft, accelerating from 100 to 130 knots over a
l0-second period just after takeoff, generates +0.16 Gx on the pilot.
Although the resultant gravitoinertial force is only 1.01 G, barely perceptibly
more than the force of gravity, it is directed 9° aft, signifying to the
unwary pilot a 9° nose-up pitch attitude. Because many slower aircraft
climb out at 6° or less, a 9° downward pitch correction would put such
an aircraft into a descent of 3° or more--the same as a normal final-approach
slope. In the absence of a distinct external visual horizon or, even worse,
in the presence of a false visual horizon (e.g., a shoreline) receding
under the aircraft and reinforcing the vestibular illusion, the pilot's
temptation to push the nose down can be overwhelming. This type of mishap
has happened at one particular civil airport so often that a notice has
been placed on navigational charts cautioning pilots flying from this
airport to be aware of the potential for loss of attitude reference.
Although the classic graveyard spiral
was indicated earlier to be a consequence of the pilot's suffering a somatogyral
illusion, it also can be said to result from a somatogravic illusion.
A pilot who is flying "by the seat of the pants" applies the necessary
control inputs to create a resultant G-force vector having the same magnitude
and direction as that which the desired flight path would create. Unfortunately,
any particular G vector is not unique to one particular condition of aircraft
attitude and motion, and the likelihood that the G vector created by a
pilot flying in this mode corresponds for more than a few seconds to the
flight condition desired is remote indeed. Specifically, once an aircraft
has departed a desired wings-level attitude because of an unperceived
roll, and the pilot does not correct the resulting bank, the only way
to create a G vector that matches the G vector of the straight and level
condition is with a descending spiral. In this condition, as is always
the case in a coordinated turn, the centrifugal force resulting from the
turn provides a Gy force that cancels the Gy component of the force of
gravity that exists when the aircraft is banked. In addition, the tangential
linear acceleration associated with the increasing airspeed resulting
from the dive provides a +Gx force that cancels the -Gx component of the
gravity vector that exists when the nose of the aircraft is pointed downward.
Although the vector analysis of the forces involved in the graveyard spiral
is somewhat complicated, a skillful pilot can easily manipulate the stick
and rudder pedals to cancel all vestibular and other nonvisual sensory
indications that the aircraft is turning and diving. In one mishap involving
a dark-night takeoff of a commercial airliner, the recorded flight data
showed that the resultant G force which the pilot created by his control
inputs allowed him to perceive his desired 10° to 12° climb angle and
a net G force between 0.9 and 1.1 G for virtually the whole flight, even
though he actually leveled off and then descended in an accelerating spiral
until the aircraft crashed nearly inverted.
Inversion Illusion
The inversion illusion is a type of somatogravic
illusion in which the resultant gravitoinertial force vector rotates
backward so far as to be pointing away from rather than toward the earth's
surface, thus giving the pilot the false sensation of being upside down.
Figure 31 shows how this can happen. Typically, a steeply climbing high-performance
aircraft levels off more or less abruptly at the desired altitude. This
maneuver subjects the aircraft and pilot to a -Gz centrifugal force
resulting from the arc flown just prior to level-off. Simultaneously,
as the aircraft changes to a more level attitude, airspeed picks up
rapidly, adding a +Gx tangential inertial force to the overall force
environment. Adding the -Gz centrifugal force and the +Gx tangential
force to the 1-G gravitational force results in a net gravitoinertial
force vector that rotates backward and upward relative to the pilot. This
stimulates the pilot's otolith organs in a manner similar to the way a
pitch upward into an inverted position would. Even though the semicircular
ducts should respond to the actual pitch downward, for some reason this
conflict is resolved in favor of the otolith-organ information, perhaps
because the semicircular-duct response is transient while the otolith-organ
response persists, or perhaps because the information from the other mechanoreceptors
reinforces the information from the otolith organs. The pilot who responds
to the inversion illusion by pushing forward on the stick to counter the
perceived pitching up and over backward only prolongs the illusion by
creating more -Gz and +Gx forces, thus aggravating the situation. Turbulent
weather usually contributes to the development of the illusion; certainly,
downdrafts are a source of -Gz forces that can add to the net gravitoinertial
force producing the inversion illusion. Again, do not assume one must
be flying a jet fighter to experience this illusion. Several reports of
the inversion illusion involve crews of large airliners who lost control
of their aircraft because the pilot lowered the nose inappropriately after
experiencing the illusion. Jet upset is the name for the sequence of events
that includes instrument weather, turbulence, the inability of the pilot
to read the instruments, the inversion illusion, a pitch-down control
input, and difficulty recovering the aircraft because of resulting aerodynamic
or mechanical forces.33
Figure
31. The inversion illusion. Centrifugal and tangential inertial
forces during a level-off combine with the force of gravity to produce
a resultant gravitoinertial force that rotates backward and upward with
respect to the pilot, causing a false percept of suddenly being upside
down. Turbulent weather can produce additional inertial forces that
contribute to the illusion. (Adapted from Martin and Jones. 32)
G-Excess Effect
Whereas the somatogravic illusion
results from a change in the direction of the net G force, the G-excess effect results from a change in G magnitude. The G-excess effect is a
false or exaggerated sensation of body tilt that can occur when the G
environment is sustained at greater than 1 G. For a simplistic illustration
of this phenomenon, let us imagine a subject is sitting upright in a +
1 Gz environment and then tips the head forward 30° (Fig. 32). As a result
of this change in head position, the subject's otolithic membranes slide
forward the appropriate amount for a 30° tilt relative to vertical, say
a distance of x JA-m. Now suppose that the same subject is sitting upright
in a +2 Gz environment and again tips the head forward 30°. This time,
the subject's otolithic membranes slide forward more than x JA-m because
of the doubled gravitoinertial force acting on them. The displacement
of the otolithic membranes, however, now corresponds not to a 30° forward
tilt in the normal 1-G environment but to a much greater tilt, theoretically
as much as 90° (2 sin 30° = sin 90°). The subject had initiated only a
30° head tilt, however, and expects to perceive no more than that. The
unexpected additional perceived tilt is thus referred to the immediate
environment, i.e., the subject perceives his or her aircraft to have tilted
by the amount equal to the difference between the actual and expected
percepts of tilt. The actual perceptual mechanism underlying the G-excess
effect is more complicated than the illustration suggests: first, the
plane of the utricular maculae is not really horizontal but slopes upward
20-30° from back to front; second, the saccular maculae contribute in
an undetermined manner to the net percept of tilt; and third, as is usually
the case with vestibular illusions, good visual orientational cues attenuate
the illusory percept. But experimental evidence clearly demonstrates the
existence of the G-excess effect. Perceptual errors of 10° to 20° are
generated at 2 G, and at 1.5 G the errors are about half that amount.34,35
Figure 32. Mechanism of G-excess
illusion. In this oversimplified illustration, the subject
in a 1-G environment (upper half of figure) experiences the result of
a 0.5-G pull on the utricular otolithic membranes when the head is tilted
30° off the vertical, and the result of a 1-G pull when the head is
tilted a full 90°. The subject in a 2-G environment (lower half of figure)
experiences the result of a 1-G pull when the head is tilted only 30°.
The illusory excess tilt perceived by the subject is attributed to external
forces (lower right). Note that the actual plane or the utricular macula
slopes 20-30° upward.
In fast-moving aircraft, the G-excess
illusion can occur as a result of the moderate amount of G force pulled
in a turn--a penetration turn or procedure turn, for example. A pilot
who has to look down and to the side to select a new radio frequency or
to pick up a dropped pencil while in a turn should experience an uncommanded
tilt in both the pitch and roll planes due to the G-excess illusion. As
noted previously, the G-excess illusion may be responsible for the false
sensation of pitch and/or roll generally attributed to the Coriolis illusion
under such circumstances. The G-excess effect has recently become a suspect
in a number of mishaps involving fighter/attack aircraft making 2- to
5.5-G turns at low altitudes in conditions of essentially good visibility.
For some reason, the aircraft were overbanked while the pilots were looking
out of the cockpit for an adversary, wingman, or some other object of
visual attention, and as a result they descended into the terrain. In
theory, the G-excess effect causes an illusion of underbank if the pilot's
head is either facing the inside of the turn and elevated (Fig. 33) or
facing the outside of the turn and depressed. If facing forward, the pilot
would have an illusion of pitching up, i.e., clubing, during the turn.
Thus, in any of these common circumstances, the pilot who fails to maintain
a continuous visual reference to the earth's surface would likely cause
the aircraft to descend in response to the illusory change of attitude
caused by the G-excess effect. Perhaps in some of the mishaps mentioned,
the pilot's view of the spatial environment was inadequate encompassing
sky rather than ground, or perhaps G-induced tunnel vision was responsible
for loss of ambient visual cues. In any case, it is apparent that the
pilots failed to perceive correctly their aircraft attitude, vertical
velocity, and height above the ground, i.e., they were spatially disoriented.
Figure 33. The G-excess illusion
during a turn in flight. G-induced excessive movement of the
pilot's otolithic membranes causes the pilot to feel an extra amount
of head and body tilt, which is interpreted as an underbank of the aircraft
when looking up to the inside of the turn. Correcting for the illusion,
the pilot overbanks the aircraft and it descends.
The elevator illusion is a special
kind of G-excess effect. Because of the way the utricular otolithic membranes
are variably displaced with respect to their maculae by increases and
decreases in +Gz force, false sensations of pitch and vertical velocity
can result even when the head remains in the normal, upright position.
When an upward acceleration (as occurs in an elevator) causes the net
Gz force to increase, a sensation of climbing and tilting backward can
occur. In flight, such an upward acceleration occurs when an aircraft
levels off from a sustained descent. This temporary increase in +Gz loading
can make pilots feel a pitch up and climb if their views of the outside
world are restricted by night, weather, or head-down cockpit chores. Compensating
for the illusory pitch up sensation, the pilot would likely put the aircraft
back into a descent, all the while feeling that the aircraft is maintaining
a constant altitude. In one inflight study of the elevator illusion, blindfolded
pilots were told to maintain perceived level flight after a relatively
brisk level-off from a sustained 2000-ft/min (10 m/sec) descent: the mean
response of the six pilots was a 1300-ft/min (6.6 m/sec) descent.15 Clearly this tendency to reestablish a descent is especially dangerous
during the final stage of a nonprecision instrument approach at night
or in weather. Upon leveling off at the published minimum descent altitude,
the pilot typically starts a visual search for the runway. In conjunction
with failing to monitor the flight instruments during this critical time,
the elevator illusion can cause the pilot to unwittingly put the aircraft
into a descent, and thus squander the altitude buffer protecting the air
Oculogravic lllusion
The oculogravic illusion can be thought
of as a visual correlate of the somatogravic illusion and occurs under
the same stimulus conditions.36 A pilot who is subjected to the deceleration resulting from the application
of speed brakes, for example, experiences a nose-down pitch because of
the somatogravic illusion. Simultaneously, the pilot observes the instrument
panel to move downward, confirming the sensation of tilting forward. The
oculogravic illusion is thus the visually apparent movement of an object
that is actually fixed relative to the subject during the changing direction
of the net gravitoinertial force. Like the oculogyral illusion, the oculogravic
illusion probably results from the attempt to maintain visual fixation
during a vestibulo-ocular reflex elicited, in this case, by the change
in direction of the applied G vector rather than by angular acceleration.
The elevator illusion was originally
thought of as a visual phenomenon like the oculogravic illusion, except
that the false percept was believed to result from a vestibulo-ocular
reflex generated by a change in magnitude of the +Gz force instead of
by a change in its direction. When an individual is accelerated upward,
as in an elevator, the increase in +Gz force elicits a vestibulo-ocular
reflex of otolith-organ origin (the elevator reflex) that drives the eyes
downward. Attempting to stabilize visually the objects in a fixed position
relative to the observer causes those objects to appear to shift upward
when the G force is increased. The opposite effect occurs when the individual
is accelerated downward; the reduction in the magnitude of the net gravitoinertial
force to less than +1 Gz causes a reflex upward shift of the direction
of gaze, and the immediate surroundings appear to shift downward. (The
latter effect also has been called the oculoagravic illusion because of
its occurrence during transient weightlessness.) Although the described
visual effect undoubtedly contributes to the expression of the elevator
illusion, it is not essential for its generation, since the illusion can
occur even in the absence of vision, as just noted.
The
Leans By far the most common vestibular
illusion in flight is the leans. Virtually every instrument-rated pilot
has had or will get the leans in one form or another at some time during
his or her flying career. The leans consists of a false percept of angular
displacement about the roll axis (i.e., an illusion of bank) and is frequently
associated with a vestibulospinal reflex, appropriate to the false percept,
that results in the pilot's actually leaning in the direction of the falsely
perceived vertical (Fig. 34). The usual explanations of the leans invoke
the known deficiencies of both otolith-organ and semicircular-duct sensory
mechanisms. As indicated previously, the otolith organs are not reliable
sources of information about the exact direction of the true vertical
because they respond to the resultant gravitoinertial force, not to gravity
alone. Furthermore, other sensory inputs can sometimes override otolith-organ
cues and result in a false perception of the vertical, even when the gravitoinertial
force experienced is truly vertical. The semicircular ducts provide false
inputs in flight by responding accurately to some roll stimuli but not
responding at all to others because they are below threshold. For example,
a pilot who is subjected to an angular acceleration in roll so that the
product of the acceleration and its time of application does not reach
some threshold value, say 2°/sec, does not perceive the roll. Suppose
that this pilot, who is trying to fly straight and level, is subjected
to an unrecognized and uncorrected 2°/sec roll for 10 seconds: a 20° bank
results. If the unwanted bank becomes suddenly apparent and is corrected
by rolling the aircraft back upright with a suprathreshold rate, the pilot
experiences only half of the actual roll motion that took place--the half
resulting from the correcting roll. As the aircraft started from a perceived
wings-Ievel position, the pilot upon returning to an actual wings-Ievel
attitude is left with the illusion of having rolled into a 20° bank in
the direction of the correcting roll and experiences the leans. Even though
the pilot may be able to fly the aircraft properly by the deliberate and
difficult process of forcing the attitude indicator to read correctly,
the leans can last for many minutes, seriously degrading flying efficiency
during that time.
Figure
34. The leans, the most common of all vestibular illusions in night. Falsely perceived to be in a right bank, but flying the aircraft straight
and level by means of the flight instruments, this pilot is leaning to
the left in an attempt to assume an upright posture compatible with the
illusion of bank.
Interestingly,
pilots frequently get the leans after prolonged turning maneuvers and
not because of alternating subthreshold and suprathreshold angular motion
stimuli. In a holding pattern, for example, the pilot rolls into a 3°/sec
standard-rate turn, holds the turn for 1 minute, rolls out and flies straight
for 1 minute, turns again for 1 minute, and so on until traffic conditions
permit the continuation of the flight toward its destination. During the
turning segments, the pilot initially feels the roll into the turn and
accurately perceives the banked attitude. But as the turn continues, the
percept of being in a banked turn dissipates and is replaced by a feeling
of flying straight with wings level, both because the sensation of turning
is lost when the endolymph comes up to speed in the semicircular ducts
(somatogyral illusion) and because the net G force being directed toward
the floor of the aircraft provides a false cue of verticality (somatogravic
illusion). Upon rolling out of the turn, the pilot's perception is of
a banked turn in the opposite direction. With experience, a pilot learns
to suppress this false sensation quickly by paying strict attention to
the attitude indicator. Sometimes, however, pilots cannot dispel the illusion
of banking--usually when they are particularly busy, unfortunately. The
leans also can be caused by misleading peripheral visual orientation cues,
as mentioned in the section entitled "Visual Illusions." Roll vection
is particularly effective in this regard, at least in the laboratory.
One thing about the leans is apparent: there is no single explanation
for this illusion. The deficiencies of several orientation-sensing systems
in some cases reinforce each other to create the illusion; in other cases,
the inaccurate information from one sensory modality for some reason is
selected over the accurate information from others to create the illusion.
Stories have surfaced of pilots suddenly experiencing the leans for no
apparent reason at all, or even of experiencing it voluntarily by imagining
the earth to be in a different direction from the aircraft. The point
is that one must not think that the leans, or any other illusion for that
matter, occurs as a totally predictable response to a physical stimulus;
there is much more to perception than stimulation of the end-organs.
|