Spatial Disorientation in FlightDynamics
The lack of adequate orientation
cues and conflicts between competing sensory modalities are only
a partial explanation of disorientation mishaps, however. Why
so many disoriented pilots, even those who know they are disoriented,
are unable to recover their aircraft has mystified aircraft accident
investigators for decades. One possibility is that the psychologic
stress of disorientation results in a disintegration of higher-order
learned behavior, including flying skills. Another is a complex
psychomotor effect of disorientation that causes the pilot to
feel the aircraft itself is misbehaving.
Visual Dominance
It is naive to assume that a certain
pattern of physical stimuli always elicits a particular veridical
or illusory perceptual response. Certainly, when a pilot has a
wide, clear view of the horizon, ambient vision supplies virtually
all necessary orientation information, and potentially misleading
linear or angular acceleratory motion cues do not result in spatial
disorientation (unless, of course, they are so violent as to cause
vestibulo-ocular disorganization). When a pilot's vision is compromised
by night or bad weather conditions, the same acceleratory motion
cues can cause spatial disorientation, but the pilot usually avoids
it by referring to the aircraft instruments for orientation information.
If the pilot is unskilled at interpreting the instruments, if
the instruments fail, or, as frequently happens, if the pilot
neglects to look at the instruments, those misleading motion cues
inevitably cause disorientation. Such is the character of visual
dominance, the phenomenon in which one incorporates visual orientation
information into a percept of spatial orientation to the exclusion
of vestibular and nonvestibular proprioceptive, tactile, and other
sensory cues. Visual dominance falls into two categories: the
congenital type, in which ambient vision provides dominant orientation
cues through natural neural connections and functions, and the
acquired type, in which orientation cues are gleaned through focal
vision and are integrated as a result of training and experience
into an orientational percept. The functioning of the proficient
instrument pilot illustrates acquired visual dominance: such an
individual has learned to decode with foveal vision the information
on the attitude indicator and other flight instruments and to
reconstruct that information into a concept of what the aircraft
is doing and where it is going, which is then used in controlling
the aircraft. This complex skill must be developed through training
and maintained through practice, and its fragility is one of the
factors that make spatial disorientation such a hazard.
Vestibular Suppression
The term vestibular suppression
often is used to denote the active process of visually overriding
undesirable vestibular sensations or reflexes of vestibular origin.
An example of this aspect of visual dominance is seen in well-trained
figure skaters who, with much practice, learn to abolish the postrotatory
dizziness, nystagmus, and postural instability that normally result
from the high decelerations associated with suddenly stopping
rapid spins on the ice. 16 But even these individuals,
when deprived of vision by eye closure or darkness, have the very
dizziness, nystagmus, and falling that we would expect to result
from the acceleratory stimuli produced.17 In flight,
the ability to suppress unwanted vestibular sensations and reflexes
is developed with repeated exposure to the linear and angular
accelerations of flight. As is the case with the figure skaters,
however, the pilot's ability to prevent vestibular sensations
and reflexes is compromised when visual orientation cues are disrupted
by night, weather, and inadequate flight instrument displays.
Opportunism
Opportunism on the part of the
primary (ambient visual and vestibular) orientation-information
processing systems refers to the propensity of those systems to
fill an orientation-information void swiftly and surely with natural
orientation information. When a pilot flying in instrument weather
looks away from the artificial horizon for a mere few seconds,
this is usually long enough for erroneous ambient visual or vestibular
information to break through and become incorporated into the
pilot's orientational percept. In fact, conflicts between focal
visual and ambient visual or vestibular sources of orientation
information tend to resolve themselves very quickly in favor of
the latter without providing the pilot an opportunity to evaluate
the information. It is logical that any orientation information
reaching the vestibular nuclei--whether vestibular, other proprioceptive,
or ambient visual--should have an advantage in competing with
focal visual cues for expression as the pilot's sole orientational
percept, because the vestibular nuclei are primary terminals in
the pathways for reflex orientational responses and are the initial
level of integration for any eventual conscious spatial orientation
percept. In other words, although acquired visual dominance can
be maintained by diligent attention to synthetic orientation cues,
the challenge to this dominance presented by the processing of
natural orientation cues through primitive neural channels is
very potent and ever present.
Disintegration of Flying Skill
The disintegration of flying skill
perhaps begins with the pilot's realization that spatial orientation
and control over the motion of the aircraft have been compromised.
Under such circumstances, the pilot pays more heed to whatever
orientation information is naturally available, monitoring it
more and more vigorously. Whether the brain stem reticular activating
system or the vestibular efferent system, or both, are responsible
for the resulting heightened arousal and enhanced vestibular information
flow can only be surmised; the net effect, however, is that more
erroneous vestibular information is processed and incorporated
into the pilot's orientational percept. This, of course, only
makes matters worse. A positive-feedback situation is thus encountered,
and the vicious circle can now be broken only with a precisely
directed and very determined effort by the pilot. Unfortunately,
complex cognitive and motor skills tend to be degraded under conditions
of psychologic stress such as occur during Type II or Type III
spatial disorientation. First, there is a coning of attention;
pilots who have survived severe disorientation have reported that
they were concentrating on one particular flight instrument instead
of scanning and interpreting the whole group of them in the usual
manner. Pilots also have reported that they were unaware of radio
transmissions to them while they were trying to recover from disorientation.
Second, there is the tendency to revert to more primitive behavior,
even reflex action, under conditions of severe psychologic stress.
The highly developed, relatively newly acquired skill of instrument
flying can give way to primal protective responses during disorientation
stress, making appropriate recovery action unlikely. Third, it
is suggested that disoriented pilots may become totally immobilized--frozen
to the aircraft controls by fear or panic--as the disintegration
process reaches its final state.
Giant Hand
The giant hand phenomenon described
by Malcolm and Money33 undoubtedly explains why many pilots have been rendered hopelessly
confused and ineffectual by spatial disorientation, even though
they knew they were disoriented and should have been able to avoid
losing control of their aircraft. The pilot suffering from this
effect of disorientation perceives falsely that the aircraft is
not responding properly to control inputs, because every attempt
to bring the aircraft to the desired attitude, seemingly is resisted
by its tendency to fly back to another, more stable attitude.
A pilot experiencing disorientation about the roll axis (e.g.,
the leans or graveyard spiral) may feel a force--like a giant
hand--trying to push one wing down and hold it there (Fig. 36),
whereas the pilot with pitch-axis disorientation (e.g., the classic
somatogravic illusion) may feel the airplane subjected to a similar
force trying to hold the nose down. The phenomenon is not rare:
one report states that 15% of pilots responding to a questionnaire
on spatial disorientation had experienced the giant hand.47 Pilots who are unaware of the existence of this phenomenon and
experience it for the first time can be very surprised and confused
by it, and may not be able to discern the exact nature of their
problem. A pilot's radio transmission that the aircraft controls
are malfunctioning should not, therefore, be taken as conclusive
evidence that a control malfunction caused a mishap: spatial disorientation
could have been the real cause.
Figure 36. The giant hand
phenomenon. This pilot, who is disoriented with respect to
roll attitude (bank angle), feels the aircraft is resisting the
attempt to bring it to the desired attitude according to the flight
instruments, as though a giant hand is holding it in the desired
attitude according to the erroneous sense of bank angle.
What mechanism could possibly
explain the giant hand? To try to understand this phenomenon,
we must first recognize that an individual's perception of orientation
results not only in the conscious awareness of position and motion
but also in a preconscious percept needed for the proper performance
of voluntary motor activity and reflex actions. A conscious orientational
percept can be considered rational, in that one can subject it
to intellectual scrutiny, weigh the evidence for its veracity,
conclude that it is inaccurate, and to some extent modify the
percept to fit facts obtained from other than the primary orientation
senses. In contrast, a preconscious orientational percept must
be considered irrational, in that it consists only of an integration
of data relayed to the brain stem and cerebellum by the primary
orientation senses and is not amenable to modification by reason.
So what happens when pilots know they have become disoriented
and try to control their aircraft by reference to a conscious,
rational percept of orientation that is at variance with a preconscious,
irrational one? Because only the data comprising one's preconscious
orientational percept are available for the performance of orientational
reflexes (e.g., postural reflexes) and skilled voluntary motor
activity (e.g., walking, bicycling, flying), it is to be expected
that the actual outcome of these types of actions will deviate
from the rationally intended outcome whenever the orientational
data on which they depend are different from the rationally perceived
orientation. The disoriented pilot who consciously commands a
roll to recover aircraft control may experience a great deal of
difficulty in executing the command, because the informational
substrate in reference to which the body functions indicates that
such a move is counterproductive or even dangerous. Or the pilot
may discover that the roll, once accomplished, must be reaccomplished
repeatedly, because of the automatic tendency to return the aircraft
to its original flight attitude in response to the preconsciously
perceived orientational threat resulting from his or her conscious
efforts and actions to regain control. Thus, the preconscious
orientational percept influences Sherrington's "final common pathway"
for both reflex and voluntary motor activity, and the manifestation
of this influence on the act of flying during an episode of spatial
disorientation is the giant hand phenomenon. To prevail in this
conflict between will and skill, the pilot must decouple voluntary
acts from automatic flying behavior. It has been suggested that
using the thumb and forefinger to move the control stick, rather
than using the whole hand, can effect the necessary decoupling
and thereby facilitate recovery from the giant hand.47
The salient features of the dynamics
of spatial orientation and disorientation are diagrammed in Figure
37 to ease the student's burden of assimilating the rather abstract
concepts discussed above. In particular, the relations between
the conscious and preconscious orientational percepts, visual
dominance, vestibular suppression, opportunism, giant hand, disintegration
of flying skill, and other aspects of orientation information
flow are presented.
Conditions Conducive to Disorientation
From knowledge of the physical
bases of the various illusions of flight, the reader can readily
infer many of the specific environmental factors conducive to
spatial disorientation. Certain visual phenomena produce characteristic
visual illusions such as false horizons and vection. Prolonged
turning at a constant rate, as in a holding pattern or procedure
turn, can precipitate somatogyral illusions or the leans. Relatively
sustained linear accelerations, such as occur on takeoff, can
produce somatogravic illusions, and head movements during G-pulling
turns can elicit G-excess illusions.
Figure 37. Flow of orientation
information in flight. The primary information-flow loop involves
stimulation of the visual, vestibular, and other orientation senses
by visual scenes and linear and angular accelerations; processing
of this primary orientation information by brain stem, cerebellum,
and cerebral centers; incorporating the solution into a data base
for reflexive and skilled voluntary motor activity (preconscious
orientational percept); and effecting control inputs, which produce
aircraft motions that result in additional orientational stimuli.
A secondary path of information flow involves the processing of
symbolic data from flight instruments into derived orientation
information by higher cerebral centers. Subloop a provides for
feedback from various components of the nervous system, and includes
efferent system influences on sensory end-organs. The phenomena
of visual dominance, vestibular suppression, and vestibular opportunism
occur in conjunction within the functioning of this loop. Subloop
b generates conscious perception of orientation, both from the
body's naturally obtained solution of the orientation problem
and from orientation information derived from flight instrument
data. Voluntary control commands arise in response to conscious
orientational percepts; and the psychic stress resulting from
conflicting orientation information or from apparently aberrantly
responding effectors can influence the manner in which orientation
information is processed, leading ultimately to disintegration
of flying skill. Subloop c incorporates feedback from muscles,
tendons, and joints involved in making control inputs, and provides
a basis for the giant hand phenomenon.
But what are the regimes of flight
and activities of the pilot that seem most likely to allow these
potential illusions to manifest themselves? Certainly, instrument
weather and night flying are primary factors. Especially likely
to produce disorientation, however, is the practice of switching
back and forth between the instrument flying mode and the visual,
or contact, flying mode; pilots are far less likely to become
disoriented if they get on the instruments as soon as out-of-cockpit
vision is compromised and stay on the instruments until continuous
contact flying is again assured. In fact, any event or practice
requiring the pilot to break an instrument cross-check is conducive
to disorientation. In this regard, avionics control switches and
displays in some aircraft are located where pilots must interrupt
their instrument cross-checks for more than just a few seconds
to interact with them, and are thus known as "vertigo traps."
Some of these vertigo traps require substantial movements of the
pilot's head during the time of cross-check interruption, thereby
providing both a reason and an opportunity for spatial disorientation
to strike.
Formation flying in adverse weather
conditions is probably the most likely of all situations to produce
disorientation; indeed, some experienced pilots get disoriented
every time they fly wing or trail in weather. A pilot who has
little if any opportunity to scan the flight instruments while
flying formation on the lead aircraft in weather is essentially
isolated from any source of accurate orientation information,
and misleading vestibular and ambient visual cues arrive unchallenged
into the pilot's sensorium,
Of utmost importance to a
pilot in preventing spatial disorientation is competency and currency
in instrument flying. A noninstrument-rated pilot who penetrates
instrument weather is virtually assured of developing spatial
disorientation within a matter of seconds, just as the most competent
instrument pilot would develop it while flying in weather without
runctioning flight instruments. Regarding instrument flying skill,
one must "use it or lose it," as they say. For that reason, it
is inadvisable and usually illegal for one to act as a pilot in.
command of an aircraft in instrument weather without a certain
amount of recent instrument flying experience.
Even the most capable instrument pilot is susceptible to spatial disorientation when attention
is diverted away from the flight instruments and' the primary
task of flying the airplane is neglected. This can happen when
other duties, such as navigation, communication, operating weapons,
responding to malfunctions, and managing inflight emergencies,
place excessive demands on the pilot's attention and lead to "task
saturation." In fact, virtually all aircraft mishaps involving
Type I spatial disorientation occur as a result of the pilot's
failure to prioritize several competing tasks properly. "First,
fly the airplane; then do other things as time allows," is always
good advice for pilots, especially for those faced with a high
mental workload. Not to prioritize in this manner can result in
disorientation and disaster .
Finally, conditions affecting
the pilot's physical or mental health must be considered capable
of rendering the pilot more susceptible to spatial disorientation.
The unhealthy effect of alcohol ingestion on neural information
processing is one obvious example; however, the less well-known
ability of alcohol to produce vestibular nystagmus (positional
alcohol nystagmus) for many hours after its more overt effects have disappeared is probably of equal significance. Use of other
drugs, such as barbiturates, amphetamines, and especially the
illegal "recreational" drugs (marijuana, cocaine, etc.) certainly
could contribute to the development of disorientation and precipitate
aircraft mishaps. Likewise, physical and mental fatigue, as well
as acute or chronic emotional stress, can rob pilots of the ability
to concentrate on the instrument cross-check and can, therefore,
have deleterious effects on their resistance to spatial disorientation. |