retroyou nostal(G) 2002 - 2003 : Spatial Disorientation in Flight
Spatial Disorientation in Flight
Definitions
Spatial disorientation is a
state characterized by an erroneous orientational percept, i.e., an
erroneous sense of one's position and motion relative to the plane of
the earth's surface. Geographic disorientation, or "being lost," is a
state characterized by an erroneous locational percept. These
definitions together encompass all the possible positions and
velocities, both translational and rotational, along and about three
orthogonal earth -referenced axes.
Orientation information
includes those parameters that an individual on or near the earth's
surface with eyes open can reasonably be expected to process accurately
on a sunny day. Lateral tilt, forward-backward tilt, angular position
about a vertical axis, and their corresponding first derivatives with
respect to time are the angular positions and motions included; height
above ground, forward-backward velocity, sideways velocity, and up-down
velocity are the linear position and motions included. Absent from this
collection of orientation information parameters are the location
coordinates, the linear position dimensions in the horizontal plane. In
flight, orientation information is described in terms of flight
instrument-based parameters (Fig. 35). Angular position is bank, pitch,
and heading; and the corresponding angular velocities are roll rate,
pitch rate, and turn rate (or yaw rate). The linear position parameter
is altitude, and the linear velocity parameters are airspeed (or
groundspeed), slip/skid rate, and vertical velocity. Inflight
navigation information is composed of linear position dimensions in the
horizontal plane, such as latitude and longitude or bearing and
distance from a navigation reference point.
Figure 35. Flight
instrument-based parameters of spatial orientation. Spatial
disorientation is a state characterized by an erroneous sense of any of
these parameters.
United States Air Force
Manual 51-37, Instrument Flying,37 categorizes
flight instruments into three functional groups: control, performance,
and navigation. In the control category are the parameters of aircraft
attitude (i.e., pitch and bank} and engine power or thrust. In the
performance category are airspeed, altitude, vertical velocity,
heading, turn rate, slip/skid rate, angle of attack, acceleration (G loading), and flight path (velocity vector). The navigation
category includes course, bearing, range, latitude/longitude, time, and
similar parameters useful for determining location on the earth's
surface. This categorization of flight instrument parameters allows us
to construct a useful operational definition of spatial
disorientation: it is an erroneous sense of the magnitude or
direction of any of the control and performance flight parameters. Geographic
disorientation, in contrast, is: an erroneous sense of any of the
navigation parameters. The practical utility of these operational
definitions is that they can establish a common understanding of what
is meant by spatial disorientation among all parties investigating an
aircraft mishap, whether they be pilots, flight surgeons, aerospace
physiologists, or representatives of some other discipline. If the
answer to the question, "Did the pilot not realize the aircraft's
actual pitch attitude and vertical velocity (and/or other control or
performance parameters)?" is "Yes," then it is obvious that the pilot
was spatially disoriented, and the contribution of the disorientation to the sequence of events leading to the mishap is clarified.
Sometimes aircrew tend to be
imprecise when they discuss spatial disorientation, preferring to say
that they "lost situational awareness" rather than "became
disoriented," as though having experienced spatial disorientation
stigmatizes them. Situational awareness involves a correct appreciation
of a host of conditions, including the tactical environment, location,
weather, weapons capability, administrative constraints, etc., as well
as spatial orientation. Thus, if the situation about which a pilot
lacks awareness is the aircraft's position and motion relative to the
plane of the earth's surface, the pilot has spatial disorientation, as
well as a loss of situational awareness, generally.
Types of Spatial
Disorientation
We distinguish three types of
spatial disorientation in flight: Type I (unrecognized), Type II
(recognized), and Type III (incapacitating). In Type I disorientation, no conscious perception of any of the manifestations
of disorientation is present; i.e., the pilot experiences no disparity
between natural and synthetic orientational percepts, has no suspicion
that a flight instrument (e.g., attitude indicator) has malfunctioned,
and does not feel that the aircraft is responding incorrectly to
control inputs. In unrecognized spatial disorientation the
pilot is oblivious to his or her disorientation, and controls the
aircraft completely in accord with and in response to a false
orientational percept. To distinguish Type I disorientation from the
others, and to emphasize its insidiousness, some pilots and aerospace
physiologists call Type I spatial disorientation "misorientation".
In Type II disorientation,
the pilot consciously perceives some manifestation of disorientation.
Pilots may experience a conflict between what they feel the aircraft is
doing and what the flight instruments show that it is doing. Or the
pilot may not experience a genuine conflict, but may merely conclude
that the flight instruments are incorrect. The pilot also may feel that
the aircraft is attempting to assume a pitch or bank attitude counter
to the intended one. Type II disorientation is the kind to which pilots
are referring when they use the term "vertigo," as in "I had a bad case
of vertigo on final approach." Although Type II spatial disorientation
is labeled "recognized," this does not mean that pilots necessarily
realize they are disoriented: they may only realize that there is a
problem in controlling the aircraft, not knowing that the source of the
problem is spatial disorientation.
With Type III spatial
disorientation, the pilot experiences an overwhelming-- i.e.,
incapacitating--physiologic response to physical or emotional stimuli
associated with the disorientation event. For example, the pilot may
suffer from "vestibulo-ocular disorganization" due to the presence of
vestibular nystagmus, so that the flight instruments cannot be read and
a stable view of the outside world cannot be obtained. Or, control of
the aircraft may be impeded by strong vestibulospinal reflexes
affecting the shoulder and arm muscles. The pilot may even be so
incapacitated by fear that rational decisions may be thwarted--e.g.,
the pilot may freeze on the controls. The important feature of Type III
disorientation is that the pilot is disoriented and most likely knows
it, but can't do anything about it.
An
orientational percept is a sense of one's linear and angular position
and motion relative to the plane of the earth's surface. It can be
primary (i.e., natural), meaning that it is based on ambient visual,
vestibular, or other sensations that normally contribute to spatial
orientation in our natural environment; or it can be secondary (i.e.,
synthetic), meaning that it is intellectually constructed from focal
visual, verbal, or other symbolic data, such as that presented by
flight instruments. While the former type of orientational percept is
essentially irrational and involves largely preconscious mental
processing, the latter type is rational and entirely conscious. A
locational percept, to be distinguished from an orientational percept,
is a sense of one's position in (as opposed to relative
to) the plane of the earth's surface. An accurate locational
percept is achieved by reading a map or knowing the latitude and
longitude of one's location.
Examples of Disorientation
The last of four F-15 Eagle
fighter aircraft took off on a daytime sortie in bad weather, intending
to follow the other three in a radar in-trail departure. Because of a
navigational error committed by the pilot shortly after takeoff, he was
unable to find the other aircraft on his radar. Frustrated, the pilot
elected to intercept the other aircraft where he knew they would be in
the arc of the standard instrument departure, so he made a beeline for
that point, presumably scanning his radar diligently for the blips he
knew should be appearing at any time. Meanwhile, after ascending to
4000 ft (1200 m) above ground level, he entered a descent of
approximately 2500 ft/min (13 m/sec) as a result of an unrecognized
3° nose-low attitude. After receiving requested position
information from another member of the flight, the pilot either
suddenly realized he was in danger of colliding with the other aircraft
or he suddenly found them on radar, because he then made a steeply
banked turn, either to avoid a perceived threat of collision or to join
up with the rest of the flight. Unfortunately, he had by this time
descended far below the other aircraft and was going too fast to avoid
the ground, which became visible under the overcast just before the
aircraft crashed. This mishap resulted from an episode of unrecognized,
or Type I, disorientation. The specific illusion responsible appears to
have been the somatogravic illusion, which was created by the forward
acceleration of this high-performance aircraft during takeoff and
climb-out. The pilot's preoccupation with the radar task compromised
his instrument scan to the point where the false vestibular cues gained
access to his perceptual processing. Having unknowingly accepted an
inaccurate orientational percept, he controlled the aircraft
accordingly until it was too late to recover .
Examples of recognized, or
Type II, spatial disorientation are easier to obtain than are examples
of Type I because most experienced pilots have anecdotes to tell about
how they "got vertigo" and fought it off. Some pilots were not so
fortunate, however. One F -15 Eagle pilot, after climbing his aircraft
in formation with another F-15 at night, began to experience difficulty
in maintaining spatial orientation and aircraft control upon leveling
off in clouds at 27,000 ft (8,200 m). "Talk about practice bleeding,"
he commented to the lead pilot. Having decided to go to another area
because of the weather, the two pilots began a descending right turn.
At this point, the pilot on the wing told the lead pilot, "Jim flying
upside down." Shortly afterward, the wingman considered separating from
the formation, saying, "I'm going lost wingman." Then he said, "No,
I've got you," and finally, "No, I'm going lost wingman." The hapless
wingman then caused his aircraft to descend in a wide spiral, and
crashed into the desert less than a minute later, even though the lead
pilot advised the wingman several times during the descent to level
out. In this mishap, the pilot probably suffered an inversion illusion
upon leveling off in the weather, and entered a graveyard spiral after
leaving the formation. Although he knew he was disoriented, or at least
recognized the possibility, he still was unable to control the aircraft
effectively. That pilots can realize being disoriented, see accurate
orientation information displayed on the attitude indicator, and still
fly into the ground always strains the credulity of nonaviators. Pilots
who have had spatial disorientation, who have experienced fighting
oneself for control of an aircraft, are less skeptical.
The pilot of an F -15 Eagle,
engaged in vigorous air combat tactics training with two other F -15s
on a clear day, initiated a hard left turn at 17,000 ft (5,200 m) above
ground level. For reasons that have not been established with
certainty, his aircraft began to roll to the left at a rate estimated
at 150 to 180°/sec. He transmitted, "Out-of-control autoroll," as
he descended through 15,000 ft (4,600 m). The pilot made at least one
successful attempt to stop the roll, as evidenced by the momentary
cessation of the roll at 8,000 ft (2,400 m); then the aircraft began to
roll again to the left. Forty seconds elapsed between the time that the
rolling began and the time that the pilot ejected--but too late.
Regardless of whether the rolling was caused by a mechanical
malfunction or was an autoroll induced by the pilot, the likely result
of this extreme motion was vestibulo-ocular disorganization, which not
only prevented the pilot from reading his instruments but also kept him
from orienting with the natural horizon. Thus, Type III disorientation
probably prevented him from taking appropriate corrective action to
stop the roll and keep it stopped; if not that, it certainly
compromised his ability to assess accurately the level to which his
situation had deteriorated.
Statistics
Because the fraction of
aircraft mishaps caused by or contributed to by spatial disorientation
has doubled over the four decades between 1950 and 1990, one might
conclude that continuing efforts to educate pilots about spatial
disorientation and the hazard it represents have been to no avail.
Fortunately, the total number of major mishaps and the number of major
mishaps per million flying hours have dropped considerably over the
same period (at least in the United States), so it appears that such
flying safety education efforts actually have been effective.
A number of statistical
studies of spatial disorientation mishaps in the United States Air
Force provide an appreciation of the magnitude of the problem in
military aviation. In 1956, Nut tall and Sanford38 reported
that, in one major air command during the period of 1954 to 1956,
spatial disorientation was responsible for 4% of all major aircraft
mishaps and 14% of all fatal aircraft mishaps. In 1969, Moser39 reported a study of aircraft mishaps in another major air command
during the four-year period from 1964 through 1967: He found that
spatial disorientation was a significant factor in 9% of major mishaps
and 26% of fatal mishaps. In 1971, Barnum and Bonner40 reviewed the Air Force mishap data from 1958 through 1968 and found
that in 281 (6%) of the 4679 major mishaps, spatial disorientation was
a causative factor; fatalities occurred in 211 of those 281 accidents,
accounting for 15% of the 1462 fatal mishaps. A comment by Barnum and
Bonner summarizes some interesting data about the "average pilot"
involved in a spatial disorientation mishap: "He will be around 30
years of age, have 10 years in the cockpit, and have 1500 hours of
first pilot/instructor-pilot time. He will be a fighter pilot and will
have flown approximately 25 times in the three months prior to his
accident." In an independent 1973 study, Kellogg41 found the
relative incidence of spatial disorientation mishaps in the years 1968
through 1972 to range from 4.8% to 6.2% and confirmed the high
proportion of fatalities in mishaps resulting from spatial
disorientation. The major (Class A) Air Force mishaps over the ten-year
period from 1980 through 1989 were reviewed by Freeman (personal
communication, 1990). He found that 81 (13%) of the 633 major mishaps
during that period, and 115 (14%) of the 795 fatalities, were due to
spatial disorientation. If we consider only the mishaps caused by
operator error, disorientation accounted for approximately one-fourth
of these (81 out of 356). If we only consider the Air Force's
front-line fighter/attack aircraft, the F-15 and F-16, nearly one-third
(26 of 86) of the losses of these aircraft resulted from spatial
disorientation. The cost of the Air Force aircraft destroyed each year
in disorientation mishaps until the decade of the 1980s was on the
order of $20 million per year. From 1980 through 1989, over $500
million dollars worth of Air Force resources were lost as a result of
spatial disorientation. Currently, the average annual dollar cost of
spatial disorientation to the Air Force is on the order of $100
million; but occasional losses of particularly expensive aircraft
result in much higher figures in some years.
Regarding the fractions of
the disorientation-related mishaps for which the various types of
spatial disorientation are responsible, the conventional wisdom is that
more than half of the mishaps involve Type I disorientation, most of
the remainder involve Type II, and very few involve Type III. The same
wisdom suggests that the source of the disorientation is visual
illusions in about half of the mishaps, and vestibular/somatosensory
illusions in the other half, with combined visual and vestibular
illusions accounting for at least some of the mishaps. An analysis of
Air Force aircraft mishaps in 1988, in which spatial disorientation was
suspected by the investigating flight surgeon, revealed that all 8
involved Type I; 2 apparently resulted from visual illusions, 3 from
vestibular illusions, and 3 from mixed visual and vestibular illusions.42
The recent experience of the
United States Navy with spatial disorientation is also instructive.43 During the years 1980 through 1989, 112 Class A flight mishaps involved
spatial disorientation as a definite, probable, or possible causal
factor. Of the 40 mishaps in the "definite" category, 20 occurred in
daytime and 20 happened at night; 17 occurred during flight over land,
and 23 resulted during flight over water. Thirty-two aircraft,
including 15 fighter/attack aircraft, 6 training aircraft, and 11 helicopters, were destroyed; and 38 lives were lost in
the 13 fatal mishaps out of the 40 Class A mishaps. The mean experience
for the Navy pilots involved in spatial disorientation mishaps was 1488
hours (median: 1152 hours), nearly the same as that for Air Force
pilots. Surprisingly, the incidence of spatial disorientation-related
mishaps for the Air Force, Navy, and Army has been remarkably similar
over the years, even though the flying missions of the several military
services are somewhat different. 44,45.
One problem with the mishap
statistics related above is that they are conservative, representing
only those mishaps in which disorientation was stated to be a possible
or probable factor by the Safety Investigation Board. In actuality,
many mishaps resulting from spatial disorientation were not identified
as such because other factors--such as distraction, task saturation,
and poor crew coordination--initiated the chain of events resulting in
the mishap; these factors were considered more relevant or more
amenable to correction than the disorientation that followed and
ultimately caused the pilot to fly the aircraft into the ground or
water. In the Air Force from 1980 through 1989, 263 mishaps and 425
fatalities, at a cost of over two billion dollars, resulted from "loss
of situational awareness" (Freeman, J .E.; personal communication,
1990). It is apparent that the great majority of those mishaps would
not have happened if the pilots had at all times correctly assessed
their pitch/bank attitude, vertical velocity, and altitude--i.e., if
they had not been spatially disoriented. Thus we can infer that spatial
disorientation causes considerably more aircraft mishaps than the
disorientation-specific incidence statistics would lead us to believe,
probably two or three times as many.
Although statistics
indicating the relative frequency of spatial disorientation mishaps in
air-carrier operations are not readily available, it would be a serious
mistake to conclude that there have been no air-carrier mishaps caused
by spatial disorientation. Fourteen such mishaps occurring between 1950
and 1969 were reportedly due to somatogravic and visual illusions that
resulted in the so-called "dark-night takeoff accident."31 In addition, 26 commercial airliners were involved in jet-upset
incidents or accidents during the same period.33 Spatial
disorientation also is a problem in general (nonmilitary,
nonair-carrier) aviation. Kirkham and colleagues 46 reported
in 1978 that although spatial disorientation was a cause or factor in
only 2.5% of all general aviation aircraft accidents in the United
States, it was the third most common cause of fatal general aviation
accidents. Of the 4012 fatal general aviation mishaps occurring in the
years 1970 through 1975, 627 (15.6%) involved spatial disorientation as
a cause or factor. Notably, 90% of general aviation mishaps in which
disorientation was a cause or factor were fatal.
Part of the process of
learning how to fly solely by reference to flight instruments, as
opposed to flying by visual reference to the outside world, involves
acquiring an ability to select and process information and to deselect
unreliable information cues. Visual dominance and vestibular
suppression are concepts of how this ability is manifested.